Score:0

Proof for secure stream cipher implies secure PRG

ru flag

I am self studying "A Graduate Course in Applied Cryptography" by Boneh-Shoup. I am not sure if my proof for the following problem in the book is correct. The problem asks to prove that if a stream cipher is semantically secure, then the underlying PRG is secure. Please let me know if my attempt works. If my solution is incorrect, I would appreciate a hint for how to approach the problem.

Let $G$ be the underlying PRG of the stream cipher $\mathcal{E}$ and let $\mathcal{B}$ be an adversary of G. We construct an adversary $\mathcal{A}$ attacking the semantic security of the corresponding stream cipher and we relate $\mathcal{A}$'s advantage to $\mathcal{B}$'s advantage.

As in the semantic security game, $\mathcal{A}$ must send messages $m_0, m_1$ to the stream cipher challenger. $\mathcal{A}$ chooses $m_0$ to be a string of zeros and samples $m_1$ at uniform random from the message space.

The challenger responds choosing $b$ at random from $\{0,1\}$, $s$ at random from the PRG seed space and computing $G(s)$. He proceeds by sending $G(s) \oplus m_b$ to $\mathcal{A}$.

Now $\mathcal{A}$ invokes $\mathcal{B}$ by playing the role of challenger to $\mathcal{B}$ in the PRG security game. He forwards $G(s) \oplus m_b$ to $\mathcal{B}$ and outputs whatever $\mathcal{B}$'s response is.

Let experiment zero be the case when $m_b$ is the string of zeros (ie b = 0). Let $W_0$ be the event where $\mathcal{B}$ outputs $1$ in experiment zero.

Let experiment one be the case when $b = 1$.Let $W_1$ be the event where $\mathcal{B}$ outputs $1$ in experiment one.

Note that $W_0$ is precisely the event where $\mathcal{A}$ outputs one in experiment zero and $W_0$ is precisely the event where $\mathcal{A}$ outputs one in experiment one. So we need to calculate $$|Pr[W_0] - Pr[W_1]|.$$ Since $m_1$ is randomly generated, the distribution of $m_1 \oplus G(s)$ is uniform random. By construction, the distribution of $m_0 \oplus G(s)$ is the same as the distribution of the PRG. So this quantity is precisely $\mathcal{B}$'s advantage.

Therefore for every PRG adversary $\mathcal{B}$ there exists a semantic security adversary $\mathcal{A}$ with $$PRGAdv(B,G) = SSAdv(A, \mathcal{E}).$$ But since the stream cipher is secure, we know $SSAdv(A, \mathcal{E})$ is negligible which tells us the PRG is secure.

Score:1
US flag

Proof looks convincing to me :)

Small nits:

Note that $W_0$ is precisely the event where $\mathcal{A}$ outputs one in experiment zero and $W_0$ [sic] is precisely the event where $\mathcal{A}$ outputs one in experiment one.

I think the second random variable here should be $W_1$, not $W_0$.

The challenger responds choosing $b$ at random from $\{0,1\}$, $s$ at random from the PRG seed space and computing $G(s)$.

This phrasing makes it seem like the encryption challenger chooses a random key/seed and bit upon receiving the encryption query consisting of two chosen messages. Usually in security games related to encryption, the challenger chooses a random key and bit once at startup, and then uses these values across every $(m_0, m_1)$ encryption query. For one-time semantic security (i.e. only needing to encrypt a single message per key), which is the relevant security definition here, this distinction doesn't matter; there is only one $(m_0, m_1)$ encryption query that the challenger responds to. It does matter for multiple-message semantic security (i.e. potentially more than one message encrypted per key), as the challenger must respond to potentially many encryption queries $(m_0, m_1)_0, (m_0, m_1)_1, \ldots$ using the same key and bit.

I sit in a Tesla and translated this thread with Ai:

mangohost

Post an answer

Most people don’t grasp that asking a lot of questions unlocks learning and improves interpersonal bonding. In Alison’s studies, for example, though people could accurately recall how many questions had been asked in their conversations, they didn’t intuit the link between questions and liking. Across four studies, in which participants were engaged in conversations themselves or read transcripts of others’ conversations, people tended not to realize that question asking would influence—or had influenced—the level of amity between the conversationalists.